1.IntroductionThe prefrontal cortex (PFC) is a pivotal region for various higher cognitive functions.1–4 Proper functioning of the PFC relies on local dopamine released by mesocortical axon terminals originating from neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA).5–9 Dysfunction in the mesocortical pathway has been implicated in several psychiatric diseases.10–12 The impact of dopamine on the local circuits of the PFC is mediated via various receptor types, including the D1 receptor and D2 receptor.6 Pharmacological and optogenetic studies have suggested distinct roles for D1 and D2 receptors.7,8,13,14 For instance, the D1 receptor, but not the D2 receptor, plays a critical role in working memory15–19 and visual attention.20 Moreover, in a risk-based decision-making task, infusion of a D1 receptor antagonist reduced risky choices, whereas a D2 receptor antagonist had the opposite effect.13 In rodents, the medial PFC (mPFC) coordinates with other areas to initiate goal-directed actions.21–25 The timing of such goal-directed actions is critical to their consequences.26 In a situation where a subject reacts to a sensory stimulus, the timing is dictated by sensory and motor processes.27,28 In contrast, in the absence of sensory triggers, the timing of self-initiated actions depends largely on internal states, leading to high variability.26,29 Previous studies, both preclinical and clinical, have shown that systemic administration of dopamine agonists or antagonists influences the timing of action initiation.30–32 However, it remains unclear whether and how the mesocortical pathway contributes to this cognitive process. Given the functional and genetic diversity of dopamine neurons,33–35 encoding various properties such as positive and negative reward prediction errors,33,36–38 motivation,39 timing-related information,40 locomotion,41–44 and motor planning/execution,34,45–49 it is crucial to identify the information conveyed by the mesocortical pathway and its impact on action initiation. Despite its importance, studies on the information conveyed by the mesocortical pathway have been scarce. Previous studies have measured dopamine concentration in the mPFC50–57 or one-photon gross calcium signals from the mesocortical projections,58,59 neither of which offers high-resolution information on individual axon terminals. One study used antidromic stimulation to identify VTA neurons projecting to the mPFC and examined their responses to noxious stimulation under anesthetized conditions.60 Recently, we developed a novel approach based on prism-mediated two-photon imaging in vivo, making it possible to visualize axon terminals in the mPFC that originate from the VTA.39 In this study, we employed this approach in mice performing a self-timed lever-press task—one type of self-initiated action where mice decide when to press the lever following the onset of an auditory cue, with a longer waiting period resulting in a larger amount of reward. We found that dopaminergic signals mediated via the D2 receptor play critical roles in determining the timing of self-initiated movements, exhibiting ramping activity immediately before action initiation. 2.Materials and Methods2.1.AnimalsAll experimental procedures were approved by the Medical University of South Carolina and Kagoshima University. C57BL/6 mice and eight heterozygous dopamine transporter (DAT)-Cre mice (, Jackson Laboratory, #006660, crossed with wild-type C57BL/6) were used in this study.39 Mice of both sexes, aged weeks, were included. The mice were maintained in group housing (up to five mice per cage) and experiments were performed during the dark period of a 12-h light/12-h dark cycle. 2.2.Headplate Implant and Virus InjectionAll surgical procedures were performed aseptically, with the mice under anesthesia with isoflurane. Lidocaine (subcutaneously at the incision) and caprofen (, intraperitoneally) were applied to prevent pain and brain edema. After surgery, the mice were allowed to recover for at least three days. No experimenter blinding was done. A custom-made headpost was glued and cemented to the skull,39,61–63 and then a small craniotomy () was performed over the VTA ( to 3.5 mm posterior and lateral from the bregma). Inside the small craniotomy, axon-GCaMP virus64 (AAV2/1-hSynapsin1-FLEx-axon-jGCaMP8m-WPRE-SV40) was volume-injected to the VTA through a pulled capillary glass (40 to ; depth: 4200 to ; ),61–63 as described previously.39 After the injection, the craniotomy was sealed with a small piece of cover glass and silicon sealant (Kwik-Cast) and the animals were returned to their home cage. 2.3.Behavioral Training in Sensory-Triggered and Self-Timed Lever-Press TasksAfter headpost implantation, mice were trained to perform a sensory-triggered lever-press task with the right forepaw (Fig. 1). Following the lever touch by the mice, after 0.5 to 2.0 s (randomized), a 9 kHz tone with a sound intensity of 70 to 75 dB was presented as a Go cue. The mice were required to press the lever within 1 s to obtain a liquid reward (sucrose water). Lever presses with longer response times were variably rewarded to maintain the motivation of the mice. If the mice released or pressed the lever before the Go cue, the trial was considered as an error. The inter-trial interval was 3 to 6 s. Once the mice learned the sensory-triggered lever-press task, we performed either window implantation together with microprism insertion (see above) or the pharmacological experiments (dopamine antagonist injections). Then, all the mice were trained for the self-timed lever-press task. After 1 to 2 weeks of sessions with the sensory-triggered task, we trained the mice on the self-timed lever-press task. In this task, when the mice touched the lever, a 14 kHz tone was presented as a warning cue. However, unlike the sensory-triggered lever-press task, the mice decided when to press the lever without a sensory instruction (i.e., without a Go cue). To encourage the mice to delay their response, we rewarded a longer response time with a larger amount of liquid reward. The relationship between the response time and the amount of liquid reward was supra-linear, with the optimum strategy involving waiting . We conducted one experimental session per day. 2.4.Microprism ImplantOnce the mice learned the sensory-triggered lever-press task (see above), a microprism65,66 was inserted for two-photon imaging as described previously.39 A rectangular craniotomy () was performed over the bilateral PFC ( to 3.5 mm anterior from the bregma), and the dura was removed over the right hemisphere. Then, a microprism implant assembly was inserted into the subdural space within the fissure. The microprism was centered anterior to the bregma to avoid damaging the bridging veins. Once implanted, the prism sat flush against the opposing fissure wall, which contained the medial wall of the PFC (mainly the prelimbic area) in the left hemisphere. The front face of the prism was oriented along the midline. The assembly consisted of a right-angle microprism (, Prism RA N-BK7, Tower Optical Corp.) and two coverslip layers (bottom layer: , top layer: ), which were glued by ultraviolet curing optical adhesive (Norland #81). The top layer of the glass was cemented to the skull with dental acrylic. The imaging was conducted on the mPFC of the left hemisphere, contralateral to the right forepaw used in the lever-press task. 2.5.Pharmacological ExperimentsOnce the mice learned the sensory-triggered and/or self-timed lever-press task, dopamine antagonists were injected into the mPFC. A small hole was made in the skull over the bilateral PFC ( to 3.5 mm anterior from the bregma) and covered with silicon sealant (Kwik-Cast). On the day of experiments, the mice were lightly anesthetized with isoflurane and dopamine antagonist (SCH23390,67 , 100 nl for the D1 receptor antagonist; eticlopride,8 , 100 nl for the D2 receptor antagonist) or phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) for control experiments was volume-injected (MO-10, Narishige) at a depth of 1.5 mm from the surface. The procedures took place for . After the injection, the craniotomy was sealed with a small piece of cover glass and silicon sealant (Kwik-Cast) and the mice started the behavioral session. We conducted one experimental session per day. Each mouse received either the D1 or D2 receptor antagonist but not both. The control experiments and dopamine antagonist injections were conducted on different days, following a randomized order. The sequence of exposures is described in Table 1 in the Supplementary Material. 2.6.In Vivo Two-Photon Calcium ImagingIn vivo two photon imaging was performed using a table-mounted microscope (MOM, Sutter Instruments) controlled by ScanImage.68 The design of the microscope and the details of the analysis are described in our previous publications.61–63 The light source was a pulsed Ti:sapphire laser (Chameleon Ultra II, Coherent), with the laser wavelength set to 980 nm, which causes a higher fluorescent change in the GCaMP signal and less scattering in the tissue than 920 nm.61,63 The laser power at the apochromatic objective lens (16×, 0.80 NA, Nikon) was , and we saw no bleaching. The imaging frame consisted of and the frame rate was approximately 30 Hz. For each imaging session, trials with response times of less than 1 s were excluded, and then sorted into three groups with equal numbers based on the response time of the mouse.28 We refer to the three groups as the short response time (RT) group, middle RT group, and long RT group. The imaging data were analyzed similarly to our previous publications61,63. To construct the heatmap shown in Fig. 3(d), the mean activity for each RT group was normalized relative to the maximum baseline activity (3.3 to 0 s before the cue onset) calculated from all the trials. The traces were transformed into percent signal change (), with the baseline for each axon defined as the 30th percentile value of all frames within a 90 s interval. The onsets of the activity for individual axons were determined as the last frame where the activity was below the baseline. The baseline was determined as the 1 s window around the time of the cue onset. 3.ResultsTo investigate the roles of the mesocortical pathway in action initiation, we developed a novel behavioral paradigm for mice: a self-timed lever-press task [Figs. 1(a) and 1(b)]. Each trial began with a warning cue (14 kHz) that signaled the start of the trial. During the trial, the mice decided by themselves when to press the lever; the longer they waited, the larger amount of reward they received. We did not provide sensory instructions as to when to initiate the actions. Although the self-timed lever-press task resulted in a larger variance of response time [Fig. 1(c) top], there was no correlation between the trial number and the response time [Fig. 1(d)]. For each trial number, the median response time across 36 mice remained consistent during the session, and there was no correlation between the trial number and the median response time (, Spearman’s rank correlation test, ). The overall response time was (). Before training the self-timed lever-press task, we trained the mice to perform a sensory-triggered lever-press task. In this task, the mice were required to press the lever as soon as a Go cue (9 kHz) was presented [Fig. 1(b)]. The sensory-triggered lever-press task led to a shorter response time (, for all mice; Wilcoxon rank sum test) and smaller variance (, for all mice; two-sample F-test for equal variances) than the self-timed lever-press task [Fig. 1(c)]. This result suggests that these movements are triggered by the sensory stimulus, with the timing of action initiation mostly dictated by sensory and motor processes.27,28. We next examined the significance of the dopamine input to the mPFC in our self-timed lever-press task and sensory-triggered lever-press task by pharmacological experiments with dopamine antagonists [Figs. 2(a) and 2(h)]. Injection of a D1 antagonist (SCH23390, , 100 nl) into the mPFC had no effect on the self-timed lever-press task (; ; Wilcoxon signed-rank test) [Figs. 2(b)–2(d)]. However, when we injected a D2 antagonist (eticlopride, , 100 nl), the response time in the self-timed lever-press task substantially increased (; ; Wilcoxon signed-rank test) [Figs. 2(i)–2(k)]. The effect of the D2 antagonist did not persist on the following day (Fig. S2-1 in the Supplementary Material). Additionally, we compared the response times of the first 50 trials of the sessions between the D2 antagonist and control experiments, and the difference persisted (Fig. S2-2 in the Supplementary Material). We injected the D2 antagonist into a neighboring area, the medial orbitofrontal cortex, which did not affect the response time (Fig. S2-3 in the Supplementary Material, ). This result demonstrated the spatial specificity of the role of D2 receptor in the self-timed lever-press task. Injection of either the D1 or D2 antagonist did not affect the animal’s performance in the control, sensory-triggered lever-press task (D1 antagonist, , ; D2 antagonist, , ; Wilcoxon signed-rank test) [Figs. 2(e)–2(g) for D1 antagonist; Figs. 2(l)–2(n) for D2 antagonist]. Thus, mesocortical projections to the mPFC play critical roles in self-timed actions via the D2 receptor. We next investigated the information conveyed via the mesocortical pathway. We injected Cre-dependent adeno-associated virus into the midbrain regions of transgenic mice (DAT-Cre57), which express Cre-recombinase in dopamine neurons58 [see Sec. 2 and Fig. 3(a)]. Our previous study confirmed that GCaMP expression in cell bodies in the VTA (and substantia nigra pars compacta) coincides with the expression of tyrosine hydroxylase, an endogenous marker for dopamine neurons.39 We imaged the axon terminals of these dopaminergic neurons in the mPFC using in vivo two-photon imaging combined with microprism insertion [Fig. 3(a)]. Unlike our previous study, we were unable to administer aversive stimuli39 because providing such stimuli would hamper the performance of the mice. Our investigation centered on determining whether mesocortical axon terminals exhibit ramping activity before action initiation and, if so, how this activity correlates with the animals’ response time [Fig. 3(b)].71,72 We specifically explored two possible scenarios regarding the ramping activity of mesocortical dopamine axons. One possibility is that the ramping activity could gradually increase after cue onset in a manner predicting response time, akin to activity patterns observed in higher motor areas71,72 [Fig. 3(b), top], reflecting the accumulation of motor plans or decisions. An alternative possibility is that the ramping activity could be initiated at a fixed time before action initiation [Fig. 3(b), bottom], suggesting that it reflects the execution of the movement. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we analyzed the activity of individual axon terminals in three groups of trials that were sorted based on the response times (green for short RT, blue for middle RT, red for late RT). We identified that a significant proportion of axon terminals exhibited ramping activity before the lever press ( out of 249 axon terminals, 8 mice)61 [Figs. 3(c) and 3(d)]. The axon terminal shown in Fig. 3(c) displayed similar activity patterns before the lever press, regardless of the response time length, consistent with scenario 2 in Fig. 3(b). This pattern held true across the population [Fig. 3(c)], and the average across 58 axons exhibited a similar activity pattern toward the execution of the lever press, whether the response time was short, middle, or long [Fig. 3(e)]. Across the population, the difference between the short and middle RT group was non-significant () for the last 330 ms before the lever press, and the difference between the middle and long RT group was non-significant for the last 1.7 s. Finally, we examined the two possible scenarios regarding how the ramp-up activity of individual axon terminals depends on response time [Fig. 3(b)]. For each axon terminal, we determined the onset time for the short, middle, and long RT groups (see Sec. 2) and plotted them against the response time of the mice in each group. Across trial groups with different response times [Fig. 3(b), right column], we found that the ramp-up time (period between the onset time and the lever press) was constant across trials [Fig. 3(f)]. To quantify this, for each axon, we computed the angle of the regression line that connects the three points corresponding to the short, middle, and long RT groups [Fig. 3(g)]. The circular mean of the angle was close to 0 deg (2.44 deg; difference from 0 deg, ; difference from , ). Therefore, we conclude that mesocortical activity initiates at a fixed time before action initiation, thus containing information related to the execution of the movements. 4.Discussion and ConclusionDopamine projections to the mPFC are recognized as crucial neuromodulators for the proper functioning of the mPFC. Various pharmacological experiments utilizing dopamine receptor blockers in the PFC have consistently induced a range of cognitive deficits.7,8,13,15–20 Despite this, the role of dopamine in the PFC in action initiation is controversial,67,69 and the information encoded by this pathway has remained unclear. Previous studies have proposed that certain dopaminergic neurons may encode action initiation.34,46,70,73 For example, the activity of neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta increases before mice transition from an immobility state to a mobility state.46 However, the previous studies did not measure the activity of neurons constituting the mesocortical projections, leaving uncertainty about whether the mesocortical pathway contributes to action initiation. Our study addresses this gap by demonstrating that the mesocortical pathway exhibits pre-movement activity and contributes to action initiation via D2 receptors in self-timed tasks, in contrast to its lack of involvement in sensory-triggered lever-press tasks. A few studies in primates have implicated the potential roles of D2 receptors in action initiation. Goldman–Rakic and her colleagues’ pioneering work showed that D2 receptors modulate neural activity in the PFC associated with saccades in a memory-guided saccade task.67 Another study employing pro- and anti-saccade tasks demonstrated that D2 receptor stimulation selectively modulated eye-movement-related activity.69 Notably, in these studies, the timing of action initiation was instructed by a sensory stimulus, and there were no67 or minimum69 effects of the D2 receptor on response time. In the present study, using a novel mouse behavioral paradigm, we demonstrate substantial effects of D2 receptor blockers in a self-timed lever-press task but not a sensory-triggered lever-press task. Furthermore, the effects were absent following D1 receptor antagonist injection, with an amount similar to that used in previous studies.74 However, we cannot completely exclude the possibility that a higher concentration of the D1 antagonist might have some effects. Our study extends the previous reports on the relationship between D2 receptors in the PFC and movement-related activity, suggesting that such activity might play a major role in self-timed actions but not in sensory-triggered actions. Our behavioral results are further supported by the existence of pre-movement activity of dopaminergic axon terminals in the PFC. Until recently, monitoring the activity of individual dopamine axon terminals in the mPFC was challenging. Our group is the first to accomplish this by combining in vivo two-photon imaging and microprism insertion.39 Importantly, our approach preserves the local circuit integrity near the imaging regions. Specifically, our mice conducted the lever press with the right forepaw, and our imaging was performed from the left mPFC in order to avoid disrupting the most relevant motor region. Our approach allowed us to uncover that different axon terminals start exhibiting pre-movement activity at different time points before the action. Interestingly, the activity patterns of individual axon terminals were very similar whether the animals responded immediately for a small amount of reward or waited longer for a larger amount of reward, indicating that the mesocortical pathway does not encode the accumulation of preparation, at least in our behavioral context. This finding provides a striking contrast with a recent study that monitored the bulk activity of the nigrostriatal pathway, where the summed activity of all the axons exhibited ramp activity immediately after the start-time cue in a self-timed task [similar to Fig. 3(b), top].48 Further studies will be required to determine whether the discrepancy between our study and the previous one might be explained by (1) the specific dopaminergic pathway (mesocortical versus nigrostriatal), (2) technical differences (individual axons vs. bulk imaging), or (3) differences in the behavioral task (timing determined by the mice in our task vs. being specified by the experimenter in the previous study). Regardless, it is still not entirely clear how such ramping activity is conveyed to the downstream circuits in the mPFC. One attractive possibility is that D2 receptors activate a specific group of layer V neurons that project subcortically.75 Alternatively, dopamine might act at D2 receptors in the interneurons to suppress inhibitory transmission.76 A promising avenue of research is to visualize the activity of D2 receptor-expressing neurons together with mesocortical axon terminals using calcium sensors of different colors. The mesocortical pathway has been implicated in various psychiatric disorders,10–12 yet technical challenges have hindered us from fully investigating the information conveyed by this pathway. The techniques we have employed in this study, together with appropriate behavioral paradigms, could lead to a better understanding of these disorders. Code and Data AvailabilityThese studies did not generate unique reagents. All data reported in this study will be shared by the corresponding authors upon request. All analysis codes from this study are available from the corresponding authors upon request. Further information and requests for reagents may be directed to the Lead Contact, Takashi R Sato (satot@musc.edu). Author ContributionsT.R.S. and T.K.S. conceived the project; M.O., K.A., M.H., and T.I. designed the experiments., H.L., T.I., T.R.S., and T.K.S. prepared the analysis codes. M.O., K.A., M.H., V.S., A.M., E.S., E.F., N.H., Y.K., and T.I. carried out experiments. M.O., K.A., M.H., V.S., T.Y., and T.K.S. analyzed the data. T.R.S. and T.K.S interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript. AcknowledgmentsThis work was supported by grants from JSPS KAKENHI to T.I. (Grant No. JP21K07459); the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (BBRF, Young Investigator Award No. 29268), National Institute on Drug Abuse (COCA pilot, Grant No. P50DA046373), National Institute of Aging (Grant No. R03 AG070517), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (Grant Nos. R21 NS125571 and R01 NS131549), BrightFocus (Grant No. A2021041S), American Heart Association (Grant No. 19IPLOI34760424), and the National Institutes of Health (NIH) COBRE in Neurodevelopment and its Disorders (Grant No. P20 GM148302) to T.R.S.; JST PRESTO (Grant No. JPMJPR1883), JST FOREST (Grant No. 23720089), and KAKENHI (Grant No. 20K23378) to T.K.S.; and JSPS Grant-Aid for Scientific Research (Grant No. 21K09105) to M.O. ReferencesE. K. Miller and J. D. Cohen,
“An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function,”
Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 24 167
–202 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167 ARNSD5 0147-006X
(2001).
Google Scholar
J. M. Fuster, The Prefrontal Cortex, Academic Press/Elsevier(
(2015). Google Scholar
E. K. Miller and J. D. Wallis, Executive Function and Higher-Order Cognition: Definition and Neural Substrates, 4 99
–104 Academic Press, Oxford
(2009). Google Scholar
R. Basu et al.,
“The orbitofrontal cortex maps future navigational goals,”
Nature, 599 449
–452 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04042-9
(2021).
Google Scholar
A. F. Arnsten, M. J. Wang and C. D. Paspalas,
“Neuromodulation of thought: flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses,”
Neuron, 76 223
–239 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038 NERNET 0896-6273
(2012).
Google Scholar
J. K. Seamans and C. R. Yang,
“The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex,”
Prog. Neurobiol., 74 1
–58 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.05.006 PGNBA5 0301-0082
(2004).
Google Scholar
M. V. Puig et al.,
“Dopamine modulation of learning and memory in the prefrontal cortex: insights from studies in primates, rodents, and birds,”
Front. Neural Circuits, 8 93 https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00093
(2014).
Google Scholar
S. B. Floresco,
“Prefrontal dopamine and behavioral flexibility: shifting from an “inverted-U” toward a family of functions,”
Front. Neurosci., 7 62 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00062 1662-453X
(2013).
Google Scholar
T. W. Robbins and A. F. Arnsten,
“The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation,”
Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 32 267
–287 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535 ARNSD5 0147-006X
(2009).
Google Scholar
F. Levy,
“The dopamine theory of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),”
Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry, 25 277
–283 https://doi.org/10.3109/00048679109077746 ANZPBQ
(1991).
Google Scholar
M. Akil et al.,
“Lamina-specific alterations in the dopamine innervation of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenic subjects,”
Am. J. Psychiatry, 156 1580
–1589 https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.10.1580
(1999).
Google Scholar
C. D. Verrico et al.,
“Pharmacotherapeutics for substance-use disorders: a focus on dopaminergic medications,”
Expert Opin. Investig. Drugs, 22 1549
–1568 https://doi.org/10.1517/13543784.2013.836488 EOIDER
(2013).
Google Scholar
J. R. St Onge, H. Abhari and S. B. Floresco,
“Dissociable contributions by prefrontal D1 and D2 receptors to risk-based decision making,”
J. Neurosci., 31 8625
–8633 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1020-11.2011 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2011).
Google Scholar
T. Nishioka et al.,
“Error-related signaling in nucleus accumbens D2 receptor-expressing neurons guides inhibition-based choice behavior in mice,”
Nat. Commun., 14 2284 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-38025-3 NCAOBW 2041-1723
(2023).
Google Scholar
J. Zahrt et al.,
“Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance,”
J. Neurosci., 17 8528
–8535 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08528.1997 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(1997).
Google Scholar
G. V. Williams and P. S. Goldman-Rakic,
“Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex,”
Nature, 376 572
–575 https://doi.org/10.1038/376572a0
(1995).
Google Scholar
T. Sawaguchi and P. S. Goldman-Rakic,
“D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: involvement in working memory,”
Science, 251 947
–950 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1825731 SCIEAS 0036-8075
(1991).
Google Scholar
Y. Chudasama and T. W. Robbins,
“Dopaminergic modulation of visual attention and working memory in the rodent prefrontal cortex,”
Neuropsychopharmacology, 29 1628
–1636 https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300490 NEROEW 0893-133X
(2004).
Google Scholar
J. K. Seamans, S. B. Floresco and A. G. Phillips,
“D1 receptor modulation of hippocampal-prefrontal cortical circuits integrating spatial memory with executive functions in the rat,”
J. Neurosci., 18 1613
–1621 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-04-01613.1998 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(1998).
Google Scholar
B. Noudoost and T. Moore,
“Control of visual cortical signals by prefrontal dopamine,”
Nature, 474 372
–375 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09995
(2011).
Google Scholar
S. Killcross and E. Coutureau,
“Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats,”
Cereb. Cortex, 13 400
–408 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/13.4.400 53OPAV 1047-3211
(2003).
Google Scholar
B. W. Balleine and A. Dickinson,
“Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates,”
Neuropharmacology, 37 407
–419 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00033-1 NEPHBW 0028-3908
(1998).
Google Scholar
M. Laubach, M. S. Caetano and N. S. Narayanan,
“Mistakes were made: neural mechanisms for the adaptive control of action initiation by the medial prefrontal cortex,”
J. Physiol. Paris, 109 104
–117 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.12.001
(2015).
Google Scholar
Q. Zhang, M. A. Weber and N. S. Narayanan,
“Medial prefrontal cortex and the temporal control of action,”
Int. Rev. Neurobiol., 158 421
–441 https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.irn.2020.11.004
(2021).
Google Scholar
N. S. Narayanan, N. K. Horst and M. Laubach,
“Reversible inactivations of rat medial prefrontal cortex impair the ability to wait for a stimulus,”
Neuroscience, 139 865
–876 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.11.072
(2006).
Google Scholar
B. Libet et al.,
“Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act,”
Brain, 106
(3), 623
–642 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623 BRAIAK 0006-8950
(1983).
Google Scholar
J. D. Schall,
“Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting,”
Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 2 33
–42 https://doi.org/10.1038/35049054 NRNAAN 1471-003X
(2001).
Google Scholar
T. R. Sato and J. D. Schall,
“Effects of stimulus-response compatibility on neural selection in frontal eye field,”
Neuron, 38 637
–648 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00237-X NERNET 0896-6273
(2003).
Google Scholar
H. H. Kornhuber and L. Deecke,
“Changes in the brain potential in voluntary movements and passive movements in man: readiness potential and reafferent potentials,”
Pflugers Arch. Gesamte. Physiol. Menschen. Tiere, 284 1
–17 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00412364
(1965).
Google Scholar
V. Voon et al.,
“Impulsive choice and response in dopamine agonist-related impulse control behaviors,”
Psychopharmacology, 207 645
–659 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-009-1697-y
(2010).
Google Scholar
M. M. van Gaalen et al.,
“Behavioral disinhibition requires dopamine receptor activation,”
Psychopharmacology, 187 73
–85 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0396-1
(2006).
Google Scholar
V. D’Amour-Horvat and M. Leyton,
“Impulsive actions and choices in laboratory animals and humans: effects of high vs. low dopamine states produced by systemic treatments given to neurologically intact subjects,”
Front. Behav. Neurosci., 8 432 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00432
(2014).
Google Scholar
M. Matsumoto and O. Hikosaka,
“Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals,”
Nature, 459 837
–841 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08028
(2009).
Google Scholar
M. W. Howe and D. A. Dombeck,
“Rapid signalling in distinct dopaminergic axons during locomotion and reward,”
Nature, 535 505
–510 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18942
(2016).
Google Scholar
M. Azcorra et al.,
“Unique functional responses differentially map onto genetic subtypes of dopamine neurons,”
Nat. Neurosci., 26 1762
–1774 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01401-9 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2023).
Google Scholar
J. Y. Cohen et al.,
“Neuron-type-specific signals for reward and punishment in the ventral tegmental area,”
Nature, 482 85
–88 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10754
(2012).
Google Scholar
W. Schultz, P. Dayan and P. R. Montague,
“A neural substrate of prediction and reward,”
Science, 275 1593
–1599 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593 SCIEAS 0036-8075
(1997).
Google Scholar
H. M. Bayer and P. W. Glimcher,
“Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal,”
Neuron, 47 129
–141 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.020 NERNET 0896-6273
(2005).
Google Scholar
K. Abe et al.,
“Enhanced aversive signals during classical conditioning in dopamine axons in medial prefrontal cortex,”
(2023). Google Scholar
S. Soares, B. V. Atallah and J. J. Paton,
“Midbrain dopamine neurons control judgment of time,”
Science, 354 1273
–1277 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aah5234 SCIEAS 0036-8075
(2016).
Google Scholar
M. W. Howe et al.,
“Prolonged dopamine signalling in striatum signals proximity and value of distant rewards,”
Nature, 500 575
–579 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12475
(2013).
Google Scholar
H. R. Kim et al.,
“A unified framework for dopamine signals across timescales,”
Cell, 183 1600
–1616.e25 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2020.11.013 CELLB5 0092-8674
(2020).
Google Scholar
K. Farrell, A. Lak and A. B. Saleem,
“Midbrain dopamine neurons signal phasic and ramping reward prediction error during goal-directed navigation,”
Cell Rep., 41 111470 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111470
(2022).
Google Scholar
P. E. Phillips et al.,
“Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking,”
Nature, 422 614
–618 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01476
(2003).
Google Scholar
P. D. Dodson et al.,
“Representation of spontaneous movement by dopaminergic neurons is cell-type selective and disrupted in parkinsonism,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 113 E2180
–2188 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1515941113
(2016).
Google Scholar
J. A. da Silva et al.,
“Dopamine neuron activity before action initiation gates and invigorates future movements,”
Nature, 554 244
–248 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25457
(2018).
Google Scholar
L. T. Coddington and J. T. Dudman,
“The timing of action determines reward prediction signals in identified midbrain dopamine neurons,”
Nat. Neurosci., 21 1563
–1573 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0245-7 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2018).
Google Scholar
A. E. Hamilos et al.,
“Slowly evolving dopaminergic activity modulates the moment-to-moment probability of reward-related self-timed movements,”
Elife, 10 e62583 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.62583
(2021).
Google Scholar
B. Engelhard et al.,
“Specialized coding of sensory, motor and cognitive variables in VTA dopamine neurons,”
Nature, 570 509
–513 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019-1261-9
(2019).
Google Scholar
E. D. Abercrombie et al.,
“Differential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortex,”
J. Neurochem., 52 1655
–1658 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989.tb09224.x JONRA9 0022-3042
(1989).
Google Scholar
C. M. Vander Weele et al.,
“Dopamine enhances signal-to-noise ratio in cortical-brainstem encoding of aversive stimuli,”
Nature, 563 397
–401 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0682-1
(2018).
Google Scholar
A. M. Thierry et al.,
“Selective activation of mesocortical DA system by stress,”
Nature, 263 242
–244 https://doi.org/10.1038/263242a0
(1976).
Google Scholar
J. M. Finlay, M. J. Zigmond and E. D. Abercrombie,
“Increased dopamine and norepinephrine release in medial prefrontal cortex induced by acute and chronic stress: effects of diazepam,”
Neuroscience, 64 619
–628 https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(94)00331-X
(1995).
Google Scholar
L. Hernandez and B. G. Hoebel,
“Feeding can enhance dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex,”
Brain Res. Bull., 25 975
–979 https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(90)90197-8 BRBUDU 0361-9230
(1990).
Google Scholar
S. Ahn and A. G. Phillips,
“Dopaminergic correlates of sensory-specific satiety in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens of the rat,”
J. Neurosci., 19 RC29 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-j0003.1999 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(1999).
Google Scholar
J. R. St Onge et al.,
“Dynamic fluctuations in dopamine efflux in the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens during risk-based decision making,”
J. Neurosci., 32 16880
–16891 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3807-12.2012 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2012).
Google Scholar
V. Bassareo, M. A. De Luca and G. Di Chiara,
“Differential expression of motivational stimulus properties by dopamine in nucleus accumbens shell versus core and prefrontal cortex,”
J. Neurosci., 22 4709
–4719 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04709.2002 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2002).
Google Scholar
C. K. Kim et al.,
“Simultaneous fast measurement of circuit dynamics at multiple sites across the mammalian brain,”
Nat. Methods, 13 325
–328 https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3770 1548-7091
(2016).
Google Scholar
I. T. Ellwood et al.,
“Tonic or phasic stimulation of dopaminergic projections to prefrontal cortex causes mice to maintain or deviate from previously learned behavioral strategies,”
J. Neurosci., 37 8315
–8329 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1221-17.2017 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2017).
Google Scholar
J. Mantz, A. M. Thierry and J. Glowinski,
“Effect of noxious tail pinch on the discharge rate of mesocortical and mesolimbic dopamine neurons: selective activation of the mesocortical system,”
Brain Res., 476 377
–381 https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(89)91263-8 BRREAP 0006-8993
(1989).
Google Scholar
M. Hasegawa et al.,
“Selective suppression of local circuits during movement preparation in the mouse motor cortex,”
Cell Rep., 18 2676
–2686 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.02.043
(2017).
Google Scholar
T. R. Sato et al.,
“Interhemispherically dynamic representation of an eye movement-related activity in mouse frontal cortex,”
Elife, 8 e50855 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.50855
(2019).
Google Scholar
T. Itokazu et al.,
“Streamlined sensory motor communication through cortical reciprocal connectivity in a visually guided eye movement task,”
Nat. Commun., 9 338 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02501-4 NCAOBW 2041-1723
(2018).
Google Scholar
G. J. Broussard et al.,
“In vivo measurement of afferent activity with axon-specific calcium imaging,”
Nat. Neurosci., 21 1272
–1280 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0211-4 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2018).
Google Scholar
R. J. Low, Y. Gu and D. W. Tank,
“Cellular resolution optical access to brain regions in fissures: imaging medial prefrontal cortex and grid cells in entorhinal cortex,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 111 18739
–18744 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1421753111
(2014).
Google Scholar
M. L. Andermann et al.,
“Chronic cellular imaging of entire cortical columns in awake mice using microprisms,”
Neuron, 80 900
–913 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.052 NERNET 0896-6273
(2013).
Google Scholar
M. Wang, S. Vijayraghavan and P. S. Goldman-Rakic,
“Selective D2 receptor actions on the functional circuitry of working memory,”
Science, 303 853
–856 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1091162 SCIEAS 0036-8075
(2004).
Google Scholar
T. A. Pologruto, B. L. Sabatini and K. Svoboda,
“ScanImage: flexible software for operating laser scanning microscopes,”
Biomed. Eng. Online, 2 13 https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-925X-2-13
(2003).
Google Scholar
S. Vijayraghavan, A. J. Major and S. Everling,
“Dopamine D1 and D2 receptors make dissociable contributions to dorsolateral prefrontal cortical regulation of rule-guided oculomotor behavior,”
Cell Rep., 16 805
–816 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2016.06.031
(2016).
Google Scholar
E. C. Syed et al.,
“Action initiation shapes mesolimbic dopamine encoding of future rewards,”
Nat. Neurosci., 19 34
–36 https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4187 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2016).
Google Scholar
D. P. Hanes and J. D. Schall,
“Neural control of voluntary movement initiation,”
Science, 274 427
–430 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.274.5286.427 SCIEAS 0036-8075
(1996).
Google Scholar
M. Murakami et al.,
“Neural antecedents of self-initiated actions in secondary motor cortex,”
Nat. Neurosci., 17 1574
–1582 https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3826 NANEFN 1097-6256
(2014).
Google Scholar
Y. Kremer et al.,
“Context-dependent multiplexing by individual VTA dopamine neurons,”
J. Neurosci., 40 7489
–7509 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0502-20.2020 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2020).
Google Scholar
A. Rinaldi et al.,
“D1 and D2 receptor antagonist injections in the prefrontal cortex selectively impair spatial learning in mice,”
Neuropsychopharmacology, 32 309
–319 https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301176 NEROEW 0893-133X
(2007).
Google Scholar
S. Gee et al.,
“Synaptic activity unmasks dopamine D2 receptor modulation of a specific class of layer V pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex,”
J. Neurosci., 32 4959
–4971 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5835-11.2012 JNRSDS 0270-6474
(2012).
Google Scholar
T. X. Xu and W. D. Yao,
“D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in separate circuits cooperate to drive associative long-term potentiation in the prefrontal cortex,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 107 16366
–16371 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1004108107
(2010).
Google Scholar
|
Axons
Neurons
Two photon imaging
Education and training
Neurophotonics
In vivo imaging
Animals